Intelligence and personality

Intelligence and personality have traditionally been studied as separate entities in psychology, but more recent work has increasingly challenged this view. An increasing number of studies have recently explored the relationship between intelligence and personality, in particular the Big Five personality traits.

General relationship[edit]

Intelligence and personality have some common features; for example, they both follow a relatively stable pattern throughout the whole of one’s life, and are to some degree genetically determined.[1][2] In addition, they are both significant predictors of various outcomes, such as educational achievement, occupational performance, and health.[3][4][5][6] The traditional view in psychology, which was that personality and intelligence should be studied as strictly separate entities, has come under scrutiny in light of modern personality research.[7]

Historically, psychologists have drawn a hard distinction between intelligence and personality, arguing that intelligence is a cognitive trait while personality is non-cognitive. However, modern psychologists argue that intelligence and personality are intertwined, noting that personality traits tend to be related to specific cognitive patterns. For example, neuroticism is a personality trait that is related to rumination and compulsive thinking about possible threats.[8] Similarly, agreeableness is a personality trait that is related to the consideration of others' mental states.[9] The finding that IQ predicts work performance, academic achievement, and health might also point to a link between intelligence and personality, or else be grounds for further research into their relationship.[10][11]

In considering the ties between intelligence and personality, it may be worth noting that they are typically not tested in the same way. Intelligence is assessed using ability tests (such as IQ tests), whereas personality is assessed using questionnaires. It has been suggested that intelligence should reflect an individual's maximal performance, while personality should reflect their typical behaviour.[12]

By personality trait[edit]

Openness[edit]

Openness shows the strongest positive relationship with g (general intelligence) among the Big Five personality traits. The main meta-analytic estimates of correlations have ranged from .17 to .23.[13][14][15] Individuals with a high level of openness enjoy the experience of learning and prefer an intellectually stimulating environment. Meta-analytic research shows that openness is more strongly related to crystallized intelligence (r = .25) than with fluid intelligence (r = .17).[14]

Some psychologists have recently pointed out that previous instruments used to measure openness actually assessed two distinctive aspects. The first is intellect, which reflects intellectual engagement and perceived intelligence and is marked by ideas, while the second is emotion, which reflects the artistic and contemplative qualities related to being engaged in sensation and perception and is marked by fantasy, aesthetics, feelings and actions.[16] Intelligence is more strongly related to intellectual engagement than with interest in aesthetics and fantasy.[14] On this basis, intellect was found to be associated with the neural system of the working memory, which is related to g, whereas openness was not.[17] In addition, according to a study of genetic behaviour, intellect is genetically closer to intelligence than openness.[18]

Neuroticism[edit]

Neuroticism has a meaningful negative correlation with intelligence. The main large meta-analyses have obtained correlations around r = −.09.[13][14] Debate exists about the extent to which the correlation reflects a substantive relationship or issues with measurement. Researchers have noted that neuroticism is correlated with test anxiety,[19][20] which refers to the psychological distress experienced by individuals prior to, or during, an evaluative situation.[21] As such, some researchers have argued that neuroticism leads to test anxiety and under performance on cognitive tests. However, others researchers have argued that test anxiety is mostly incidental and caused by neuroticism, contextual pressures, and lower cognitive ability.[14][22]

According to the results of a longitudinal study conducted by Gow et al., (2005), neuroticism influences an age-related decline in intelligence and there is a small negative correlation between neuroticism and a change in the level of IQ (r = −.18).[23] Although it is still debatable if neuroticism reduces general intelligence, this study provided some valuable evidence and a direction for research. In addition, some interaction between intelligence and neuroticism has been found. Individuals with a high level of neuroticism demonstrated a poor performance, health, and adjustment only if they had a low level of intelligence.[24] Therefore, intelligence may act as a buffer against the negative effects of neuroticism in individuals.

Conscientiousness[edit]

The association between conscientiousness and intelligence is complex and uncertain. Some researchers theorize that people with lower levels of intelligence compensate for their lower level of cognitive ability by being more structured and effortful.[25] While some studies have obtained negative correlations,[6][25][26][13][27] others have not.[28][29] In particular, a meta-analysis of 214 studies (n = 120,885) obtained a correlation of r = −.02.[14] A meta-analysis of facet-level correlations suggested that preference for order and self-discipline may have a small negative correlation whereas conscientiousness facets related to competence have a small positive correlation with intelligence.[14]

Furthermore, some interaction has been found between conscientiousness and intelligence. Conscientiousness has been found to be a stronger predictor of safety behaviour in individuals with a low level of intelligence than in those with a high level.[30] This interaction may also be found in educational and occupational settings in future studies. Therefore, relatively speaking, an increase in either conscientiousness or intelligence may compensate for a deficiency in the other.

Extraversion[edit]

Most meta-analyses have found no correlation between overall extraversion and intelligence.[14][15][13] There is some meta-analytic evidence to suggest that aspects of extraversion related to assertiveness may be slightly positively related to intelligence whereas aspects related to sociability may be slightly negatively related to intelligence.[31][14]

There are some moderating variables in the relationship between extraversion and g including differences in the assessment instruments and samples’ age and sensory stimulation; for example, no meaningful correlation was found between extraversion and intelligence in the samples of children.[32][33] Furthermore, Bates and Rock (2004) used Raven’s matrices and found that extraverts performed better than introverts with increasing auditory stimulation,[34] whereas introverts performed best in silence. This result is consistent with that of Revelle et al. (1976).[35]

Agreeableness[edit]

Meta-analytic research suggests that agreeableness and intelligence are uncorrelated.[29][13][14] However, some components of agreeableness have been found to be related to intelligence. For example, aggression is negatively associated with intelligence (r is around −.20) [29][36][37] because unintelligent people may experience more frustration, which may lead to aggression[38] and aggression and intelligence may share some biological factors.[39] In addition, emotional perception and emotional facilitation, which are also components of agreeableness, have been found to be significantly correlated with intelligence.[40][41] This may be because emotional perception and emotional facilitation are components of emotional intelligence and some researchers have found that emotional intelligence is a Second-Stratum Factor of g.[42] Similarly, meta-analysis suggests that the related trait of honesty-humility is also uncorrelated with intelligence.[14]

References[edit]

  1. ^ Caspi, A. (2000). "The child is father of the man: personality continuities from childhood to adulthood". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 158–172. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.589.3665. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.158. PMID 10653512.
  2. ^ Deary, I. J.; Whalley, L. J.; Lemmon, H.; Crawford, J. R.; Starr, J. M. (2000). "The stability of individual differences in mental ability from childhood to old age: follow-up of the 1932 Scottish Mental Survey". Intelligence. 28 (1): 49–55. doi:10.1016/s0160-2896(99)00031-8.
  3. ^ Barrick, M. R.; Mount, M. K. (1991). "The Big Five personality dimensions and job performance: A meta - analysis". Personnel Psychology. 44: 1–26. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.1991.tb00688.x. S2CID 144689146.
  4. ^ Calvin, C.; Batty, G. D.; Deary, I. J. (2011). Cognitive epidemiology: Concepts, evidence, and future directions. Oxford:Wiley–Blackwell.
  5. ^ Kern, M.; Friedman, H. (2011). Personality and health. Oxford:Wiley–Blackwell.
  6. ^ a b Poropat, A. (2009). "A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic performance". Psychological Bulletin. 135 (2): 322–338. doi:10.1037/a0014996. hdl:10072/30324. PMID 19254083.
  7. ^ Kaufman, S. B. (April 21, 2014). "How Does IQ Relate to Personality?". Scientific American.
  8. ^ Nolan, S. A.; Roberts, J. E.; Gotlib, I. H. (1998). "Neuroticism and ruminative response style as predictors of change in depressive symptomatology". Cognitive Therapy and Research. 22 (5): 445–455. doi:10.1023/a:1018769531641. S2CID 15419457.
  9. ^ Nettle, D.; Liddle, B. (2008). "Agreeableness is related to social-cognitive, but not social-perceptual, theory of mind". European Journal of Personality. 22 (4): 323–335. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.594.7750. doi:10.1002/per.672. S2CID 51682037.
  10. ^ Sternberg, R. J.; Grigorenko, E. L. The general factor of intelligence: How general is it?. Mahwah: NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 331–380.
  11. ^ Gottfredson, L. S.; Deary, I. J. (2004). "Intelligence predicts health and longevity, but why?". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 13: 1–4. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301001.x. S2CID 15176389.
  12. ^ Cronbach, L. J. (1949). "Essentials of psychological testing". {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  13. ^ a b c d e Judge, T. A.; Jackson, C. L.; Shaw, J. C.; Scott, B. A.; Rich, B. L. (2007). "Self-efficacy and work-related performance: the integral role of individual differences". Journal of Applied Psychology. 92 (1): 107–127. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.1.107. PMID 17227155.
  14. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k Anglim, Jeromy; Dunlop, Patrick D.; Wee, Serena; Horwood, Sharon; Wood, Joshua K.; Marty, Andrew (2022). "Personality and intelligence: A meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 148 (5–6): 301–336. doi:10.1037/bul0000373. hdl:20.500.11937/89668. ISSN 1939-1455. S2CID 253344751.
  15. ^ a b Stanek, Kevin (2014). "Meta-Analyses of Personality and Cognitive Ability". PhD Thesis. hdl:11299/201107.
  16. ^ DeYoung, C. G.; Quilty, L. C.; Peterson, J. B. (2007). "Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 93 (5): 880–896. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880. PMID 17983306. S2CID 8261816.
  17. ^ DeYoung, C. G.; Shamosh, N. A.; Green, A. E.; Braver, T. S.; Gray, J. R. (2009). "Intellect as distinct from Openness: Differences revealed by fMRI of working memory". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 97 (5): 883–892. doi:10.1037/a0016615. PMC 2805551. PMID 19857008.
  18. ^ Wainwright, M. A.; Wright, M. J.; Luciano, M.; Geffen, G. M.; Martin, N. G. (2008). "Genetic covariation among facets of openness to experience and general cognitive ability " (PDF). Twin Research and Human Genetics. 11 (3): 275–286. doi:10.1375/twin.11.3.275. hdl:20.500.11820/142f022a-ee86-4c68-b327-7c73601f0e34. PMID 18498206. S2CID 9994912.
  19. ^ Chamorro - Premuzic, T.; Ahmetoglu, G.; Furnham, A. (2008). "Little more than personality:Trait determinants of test anxiety ". Learning and Individual Differences. 18 (2): 258–263. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2007.09.002.
  20. ^ Wicherts, J. M.; Scholten, A. (2010). "Test anxiety and the validity of cognitive tests: A confirmatory factor analysis perspective and some empirical findings". Intelligence. 38: 169–178. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2009.09.008.
  21. ^ Zuckerman, M.; Spielberger, C. D. Emotions and anxiety: New concepts, methods, and applications. New York:Wiley. pp. 57–81.
  22. ^ Sommer, Markus; Arendasy, Martin E. (2014-01-01). "Comparing different explanations of the effect of test anxiety on respondents' test scores". Intelligence. 42: 115–127. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.11.003. ISSN 0160-2896.
  23. ^ Gow, A. J.; Whiteman, M. C.; Pattie, A.; Deary, I. J. (2005). "The personality–intelligence interface: Insights from an ageing cohort". Personality and Individual Differences. 39 (4): 751–761. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.01.028.
  24. ^ Leikas, S.; M¨akinen, S.; L¨ onnqvist, J.E.; Verkasalo, M. (2009). "Cognitive ability×Emotional stability interactions on adjustment". European Journal of Personality. 23 (4): 329–342. doi:10.1002/per.711. S2CID 145265142.
  25. ^ a b Chamorro-Premuzic, T.; Moutafi, J.; Furnham, A. (2005). "The relationship between personality traits, subjectively-assessed and fluid intelligence". Personality and Individual Differences. 38 (7): 1517–1528. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.09.018.
  26. ^ Moutafi, J.; Furnham, A.; Crump, J. (2003). "Demographic and personality predictors of intelligence: A study using the NEO Personality Inventory and the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator". European Journal of Personality. 17: 79–94. doi:10.1002/per.471. S2CID 143599504.
  27. ^ Moutafi, J.; Furnham, A.; Crump, J. (2006). "What facets of openness and conscientiousness predict fluid intelligence score?". Learning and Individual Differences. 16: 31–42. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2005.06.003.
  28. ^ Austin=, A. J.; Deary, I. J.; Whiteman, M. C.; Fowkes, F. G. R.; Padersen, N. L.; Rabbitt, P.; Bent, N.; McInnes, L. (2002). "Relationships between ability and personality: Does intelligence contribute positively to personal and social adjustment?". Personality and Individual Differences. 32 (8): 1391–1411. doi:10.1016/s0191-8869(01)00129-5.
  29. ^ a b c Ackerman, P. L.; Heggestad, E. D. (1997). "Intelligence, personality, and interests: Evidence for overlapping traits". Psychological Bulletin. 121 (2): 219–245. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.2.219. PMID 9100487.
  30. ^ Postlethwaite, B.; Robbins, S.; Rickerson, J.; McKinniss, T. (2009). "The moderation of conscientiousness by cognitive ability when predicting workplace safety behaviour". Personality and Individual Differences. 47 (7): 711–716. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.008.
  31. ^ Wolf, M. B.; Ackerman, P. L. (2005). "Extraversion and intelligence: A meta-analytic investigation". Personality and Individual Differences. 39 (3): 531–542. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.020. hdl:1853/9454.
  32. ^ Rawlings, D.; Skok, M. (1993). "Extraversion, venturesomeness and intelligence in children". Personality and Individual Differences. 15 (4): 389–396. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(93)90066-c.
  33. ^ Saklofske, D. H.; Kostura, D. D. (1990). "Extraversion–introversion and intelligence". Personality and Individual Differences. 11 (6): 547–551. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(90)90036-q.
  34. ^ Bates, T. C.; Rock, A. (2004). "Personality and information processing speed: Independent influences on intelligent performance". Intelligence. 32 (1): 33–46. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2003.08.002.
  35. ^ Revelle, W.; Amaral, P.; Turriff, S. (1976). "Introversion/extraversion, time stress, and caffeine". Science. 192 (4235): 149–150. doi:10.1126/science.1257762. PMID 1257762.
  36. ^ DeYoung, C. G.; Peterson, J. B.; S´eguin, J. R.; Pihl, R. O.; Tremblay, R. E. (2008). "Externalizing behavior and the higher-order factors of the Big Five". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 117 (4): 947–953. doi:10.1037/a0013742. PMID 19025240. S2CID 14737521.
  37. ^ Séguin, J. R.; Boulerice, B.; Harden, P.; Tremblay, R. E.; Pihl, R. O. (1999). "Executive functions and physical aggression after controlling for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, general memory, and IQ". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 40 (8): 1197–1208. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.565.303. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00536. PMID 10604398.
  38. ^ Lynam, D. R.; Moffitt, T. E.; Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1993). "Explaining the relation between IQ and delinquency: Class, race, test motivation, school failure, or self-control?". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 102 (2): 187–196. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.102.2.187. PMID 8315131.
  39. ^ DeYoung, C. G.; Peterson, J. B.; Séguin, J. R.; Mejia, J. M.; Pihl, R. O.; Beitchman, J. H.; Jain, U.; Tremblay, R. E.; Kennedy, J. L.; Palmour, R. M. (2006). "The dopamine D4 receptor gene and moderation of the association between externalizing behavior and IQ". Archives of General Psychiatry. 63 (12): 1410–1416. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.63.12.1410. PMC 3283582. PMID 17146015.
  40. ^ Mayer, J. D.; Salovey, P.; Caruso, D. R. (2004). "Emotional intelligence: Theory, findings, and implications". Psychological Inquiry. 60 (3): 197–215. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1503_02. S2CID 144415437.
  41. ^ Mayer, J. D.; Salovey, P.; Caruso, D. R. (2008). "Emotional intelligence: New ability or eclectic traits?". American Psychologist. 63 (6): 503–517. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.453.6438. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.63.6.503. PMID 18793038.
  42. ^ MacCann, C.; Joseph, D. L.; Newman, D. A.; Roberts, R. D. (2014). "Emotional intelligence is a second-stratum factor of intelligence: Evidence from hierarchical and bifactor models". Emotion. 14 (2): 358–374. doi:10.1037/a0034755. PMID 24341786.